Difference between revisions of "1080: Visual Field"

Explain xkcd: It's 'cause you're dumb.
Jump to: navigation, search
m (nav header)
(A bullet list with one entry???)
(25 intermediate revisions by 13 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{ComicHeader|1080|7/16/12}}
+
{{comic
 +
| number    = 1080
 +
| date      = July 11, 2012
 +
| title    = Visual Field
 +
| image    = visual_field.png
 +
| imagesize = 800px
 +
| titletext = I recently learned something that solved a mystery that had bugged me since childhood--why, when I looked at an analog clock, the hand would sometimes seem to take a couple seconds to start ticking. Google "stopped clock illusion".
 +
}}
 +
A [https://xkcd.com/1080/large/ larger version] of this image can be found by clicking the image at xkcd.com - the comic's page can also be accessed by clicking on the comic number above.
  
[[File:visual_field.png]]
+
==Explanation==
 +
This comic shows a number of vision related facts, arranged in a way that they all fit inside your {{w|field of vision}} (the conic area in which you can see at any given time). You're supposed to look at the center of the image while standing about a foot away from the screen (although obviously you can't read the text on the image while staring at the center).
  
== Image Text ==
+
Firstly, there's detail. The eye always sees objects closer to the eye with more detail, which [[Randall]] illustrates with progressively smaller images, which are seen with the same level of detail (remember that you're supposed to be looking at the center of the image). This is because the {{w|retina}} is denser near the {{w|fovea}}, in the center.
I recently learned something that solved a mystery that had bugged me since childhood--why, when I looked at an analog clock, the hand would sometimes seem to take a couple seconds to start ticking. Google "stopped clock illusion".
 
  
== Description ==
+
Next, there's the topic of {{w|night vision}}. The colour-seeing {{w|cone cells}} don't work so well in the dark, whereas the black-and-white-seeing {{w|rod cells}} do. The rod cells can see shapes well, whereas the cone cells see detail (such as change in colour), which Randall uses to explain why we can't read at night.
There's a lot of informative detail here, so click on the image to see the large version.
 
  
Stopped Clock Illusion or Chronostatis:[http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Chronostasis]
+
{{w|Polarization (waves)|Polarization}} direction can be visible when quickly changing your viewing angle. Polarization is essentially the vertical direction of waves. Light, being a wave, has a direction, and is thus polarized. Polarized lenses, for example, would have "slits" to allow only light that is polarized in a certain direction to come through (blocking the light in other directions). {{w|LCD}} screens operate on the principle of blocking and rotating polarized light.
  
    The term chronostasis refers to the illusion in which the first impression following a saccade (quick eye movement) appears to be extended in time.  
+
{{w|Floater|Floaters}} are deposits within the eye's {{w|vitreous humour}}. While normally transparent, they can occasionally cause {{w|refraction}} of light, making them visible, particularly on bright, blue surfaces. Randall points out that while some floaters are caused by breakdown over time, the others have a more mysterious origin.
    The most well-known version of this illusion is the stopped-clock illusion where the first movement of the second hand of an analogue clock,
 
    following the viewer directing attention to the clock, appears to take longer than the next movement.
 
    When eyes move from one point to another (a saccade), perception of time stretches slightly backward.  
 
    The brain tells us that we've been looking at the watch for slightly longer than we really have, hence the illusion that the
 
    second-hand is frozen for more than a second. Although this happens every time our eyes move from one fixation point
 
    to the next, we rarely notice it. One explanation is that our brains are filling in the gap while our eyes move from looking at one thing to the next.
 
    Experiments have shown that this illusion is probably caused by the way in which the brain attempts to construct a continuous consciousness experience in spite of
 
    quick eye movements (saccades). Although this effect is present with all eye movements, it is most noticeable when an external time-keeping source is observed.
 
  
In the bottom left, the "T-Boz Blind Spot" and the "Chilli Blind Spot" are linked to the "Left-Eye Blind Spot", which all three are members of the 90s pop trio, TLC, who's most famous songs were "Creep" and "Waterfalls".
+
Blue sky sprites, properly known as the {{w|blue field entoptic phenomenon}}, are bright sprites seen over bright blue surfaces, particularly the sky. They are {{w|white blood cells}} moving in front of the {{w|retina}}.
[[Category:Comics|1080]]
+
 
 +
Randall also points out that colours are mostly seen near the center of our vision, with our brain keeping track of the colours of things near the outside of our visual field. The cones of blue, red and green in the {{w|Quadrant (plane geometry)|third quadrant}} also show how red and green's sensitivity is mostly limited to the center of our vision, whereas we can see blue in a larger field of vision. Our ability to perceive {{w|saturation}} (the intensity of colours) is also stronger near the center of our vision.
 +
 
 +
The left and right blind spot are the locations of the {{w|optic disc}}, where there are no sensitive rod or cone cells, making a literal "blind" spot. The mention of the "T-Boz blind spot" and "Chilli blind spot" are a reference to the R&B band {{w|TLC (band)|TLC}}, whose members go by the aliases "Left eye", "T-Boz", and "Chilli".
 +
 
 +
An image of the moon and a supermoon also appear in the image. A {{w|supermoon}} is when the moon is at its closest approach to Earth and coincides with a {{w|full moon}} or {{w|new moon}}, causing it to appear larger than normal. At the sizes Randall has drawn the two moons, the difference in size (approximately ten percent) is nigh-imperceptible to the naked eye; Randall seems to be making a comment about how supermoons aren't impressive to him. That he feels like this was already indicated in [[1052: Every Major's Terrible#Verse 3|panel 25]] of [[1052: Every Major's Terrible]] and then later confirmed when he published [[1394: Superm*n]]. Here is [[:Category:Supermoon|a list]] of all comics referring to the term.
 +
 
 +
The "stopped clock illusion" referenced by the image text is an example of {{w|chronostasis}}, which is an {{w|illusion}} where viewing movement after changing your vision is perceived as taking a longer period of time. So when we look at a clock (which we weren't previously looking at), our field of vision has rapidly changed. The second hand on the clock thus seems to take a longer period of time to move.
 +
 
 +
==Transcript==
 +
:Your Central Visual Field
 +
:[This comic contains numerous visual elements arranged around a central point, and are intended to represent locations in a sphere with the eyeball as the center. Underlaid below all of the elements are concentric circles representing degrees from straight ahead, using the eyeball's point of view, denoting where these elements would appear in someone's field of vision given proper setup. For this description, elements will be described using this grid plus location in degrees within the specified circle, placing 0 degrees to the right and going counterclockwise, separated with the word "mark".]
 +
:[At the top are the instructions to view this page]
 +
:Look at the center with your eyes this far from the screen.
 +
:[A rolled-up sheet of paper that equals about 55 total horizontal degrees in width in the measurement of the chart.]
 +
:(You can roll up a sheet of paper and cut it - or zoom the page - so it matches this image)
 +
:17 mark 0: right eye blind spot.
 +
:from 0 to 30 mark 15:
 +
:[The same image, increasing in absolute size from a very tiny object in the center to one about 20x original size at 30 degrees.]
 +
:Detail - We only see at high resolution over a small area in the center of our vision where retinal cells are densest (the fovea). If you stare at the center of this chart, your eyes are seeing all these panels at roughly the same level of detail.
 +
:9 mark 105: Moon.
 +
:7 mark 112: Supermoon.
 +
:from 0 to 20 mark 170:
 +
:[Sets 3 partially overlapping circles in multiple locations along this path. Each set has a primary color in each circle and additive colors in the overlap areas, with color saturation decreasing sharply as the sets leave the center.]
 +
:Color Vision: We don't see much color outside the center of our vision - our brains keep track of what color things are and fill it in for us.
 +
:17 mark 180: Left Eye Blind Spot.
 +
:(not pictured: T-Boz blind spot, Chilli blind spot)
 +
:From 0 to infinity mark from 180 to 205:
 +
:[A swath of blue, with heavier saturation up to 5 degrees from center to fading, but never gone out to the edges of the image.]
 +
:From 0 to 7 from 205 to 235:
 +
:[A swath of red, with full saturation in the center and fading out completely at 7 degrees from center.]
 +
:From 0 to 7 from 235 to 270:
 +
:[A swath of green, with full saturation in the center and fading out completely at 7 degrees from center.]
 +
:Red and green-sensitive cones are mainly limited to the center of our vision. We have few blue-sensitive cone cells, but they're found out to the edge of our vision.
 +
:25 mark 205: [A small whisp of white in a swath of blue.]
 +
:Blue-sky sprites: These tiny, darting spots, visible against smooth blue backgrounds, are white cells moving in the blood vessels over the retina
 +
:5 mark 195:
 +
:[A long blob, slightly distorting the blue swath.]
 +
:Floaters: Some types of floaters are caused by breakdown of your eyeball goop as you age, but this type is some other kind of debris near the retina. I don't know what.
 +
:10 mark 270:
 +
:[An askew crosshair and circle, with faint blue and yellow wedges inside]
 +
:Humans can see polarization - Stare at a white area on an LCD display while rotating it or your head fast (use straight ahead as the axis of rotation).
 +
:Polarization direction is shown by a faint central yellow blue shape (Also visible in deep blue skies)
 +
:from 0 to 30 mark 340:
 +
:[The same image, increasing in absolute size from a very tiny object in the center to one about 20x original size at 30 degrees. The brightness of the image varies from black at 2 mark 340, to gray at 5 mark 340, to nearly white at 10 mark 340, to slightly grayer at 20 mark 340, to medium gray at 30 mark 340.]
 +
:Night Vision: Cone cells (sharp, central color vision) don't work in low light, but rod cells (monochrome, low-res, non-central) do. This is why you can walk around in dim light, but not read. It's also why you can spot fainter stars by looking next to them.
 +
 
 +
{{Comic discussion}}
 +
[[Category:Comics with color]]
 +
[[Category:Large drawings]]
 +
[[Category:Charts]]
 +
[[Category:Supermoon]]
 +
[[Category:Biology]]
 +
[[Category:Physics]]

Revision as of 20:58, 11 January 2018

Visual Field
I recently learned something that solved a mystery that had bugged me since childhood--why, when I looked at an analog clock, the hand would sometimes seem to take a couple seconds to start ticking. Google "stopped clock illusion".
[Click comic to enlarge]
Title text: I recently learned something that solved a mystery that had bugged me since childhood--why, when I looked at an analog clock, the hand would sometimes seem to take a couple seconds to start ticking. Google "stopped clock illusion".

A larger version of this image can be found by clicking the image at xkcd.com - the comic's page can also be accessed by clicking on the comic number above.

Explanation

This comic shows a number of vision related facts, arranged in a way that they all fit inside your field of vision (the conic area in which you can see at any given time). You're supposed to look at the center of the image while standing about a foot away from the screen (although obviously you can't read the text on the image while staring at the center).

Firstly, there's detail. The eye always sees objects closer to the eye with more detail, which Randall illustrates with progressively smaller images, which are seen with the same level of detail (remember that you're supposed to be looking at the center of the image). This is because the retina is denser near the fovea, in the center.

Next, there's the topic of night vision. The colour-seeing cone cells don't work so well in the dark, whereas the black-and-white-seeing rod cells do. The rod cells can see shapes well, whereas the cone cells see detail (such as change in colour), which Randall uses to explain why we can't read at night.

Polarization direction can be visible when quickly changing your viewing angle. Polarization is essentially the vertical direction of waves. Light, being a wave, has a direction, and is thus polarized. Polarized lenses, for example, would have "slits" to allow only light that is polarized in a certain direction to come through (blocking the light in other directions). LCD screens operate on the principle of blocking and rotating polarized light.

Floaters are deposits within the eye's vitreous humour. While normally transparent, they can occasionally cause refraction of light, making them visible, particularly on bright, blue surfaces. Randall points out that while some floaters are caused by breakdown over time, the others have a more mysterious origin.

Blue sky sprites, properly known as the blue field entoptic phenomenon, are bright sprites seen over bright blue surfaces, particularly the sky. They are white blood cells moving in front of the retina.

Randall also points out that colours are mostly seen near the center of our vision, with our brain keeping track of the colours of things near the outside of our visual field. The cones of blue, red and green in the third quadrant also show how red and green's sensitivity is mostly limited to the center of our vision, whereas we can see blue in a larger field of vision. Our ability to perceive saturation (the intensity of colours) is also stronger near the center of our vision.

The left and right blind spot are the locations of the optic disc, where there are no sensitive rod or cone cells, making a literal "blind" spot. The mention of the "T-Boz blind spot" and "Chilli blind spot" are a reference to the R&B band TLC, whose members go by the aliases "Left eye", "T-Boz", and "Chilli".

An image of the moon and a supermoon also appear in the image. A supermoon is when the moon is at its closest approach to Earth and coincides with a full moon or new moon, causing it to appear larger than normal. At the sizes Randall has drawn the two moons, the difference in size (approximately ten percent) is nigh-imperceptible to the naked eye; Randall seems to be making a comment about how supermoons aren't impressive to him. That he feels like this was already indicated in panel 25 of 1052: Every Major's Terrible and then later confirmed when he published 1394: Superm*n. Here is a list of all comics referring to the term.

The "stopped clock illusion" referenced by the image text is an example of chronostasis, which is an illusion where viewing movement after changing your vision is perceived as taking a longer period of time. So when we look at a clock (which we weren't previously looking at), our field of vision has rapidly changed. The second hand on the clock thus seems to take a longer period of time to move.

Transcript

Your Central Visual Field
[This comic contains numerous visual elements arranged around a central point, and are intended to represent locations in a sphere with the eyeball as the center. Underlaid below all of the elements are concentric circles representing degrees from straight ahead, using the eyeball's point of view, denoting where these elements would appear in someone's field of vision given proper setup. For this description, elements will be described using this grid plus location in degrees within the specified circle, placing 0 degrees to the right and going counterclockwise, separated with the word "mark".]
[At the top are the instructions to view this page]
Look at the center with your eyes this far from the screen.
[A rolled-up sheet of paper that equals about 55 total horizontal degrees in width in the measurement of the chart.]
(You can roll up a sheet of paper and cut it - or zoom the page - so it matches this image)
17 mark 0: right eye blind spot.
from 0 to 30 mark 15:
[The same image, increasing in absolute size from a very tiny object in the center to one about 20x original size at 30 degrees.]
Detail - We only see at high resolution over a small area in the center of our vision where retinal cells are densest (the fovea). If you stare at the center of this chart, your eyes are seeing all these panels at roughly the same level of detail.
9 mark 105: Moon.
7 mark 112: Supermoon.
from 0 to 20 mark 170:
[Sets 3 partially overlapping circles in multiple locations along this path. Each set has a primary color in each circle and additive colors in the overlap areas, with color saturation decreasing sharply as the sets leave the center.]
Color Vision: We don't see much color outside the center of our vision - our brains keep track of what color things are and fill it in for us.
17 mark 180: Left Eye Blind Spot.
(not pictured: T-Boz blind spot, Chilli blind spot)
From 0 to infinity mark from 180 to 205:
[A swath of blue, with heavier saturation up to 5 degrees from center to fading, but never gone out to the edges of the image.]
From 0 to 7 from 205 to 235:
[A swath of red, with full saturation in the center and fading out completely at 7 degrees from center.]
From 0 to 7 from 235 to 270:
[A swath of green, with full saturation in the center and fading out completely at 7 degrees from center.]
Red and green-sensitive cones are mainly limited to the center of our vision. We have few blue-sensitive cone cells, but they're found out to the edge of our vision.
25 mark 205: [A small whisp of white in a swath of blue.]
Blue-sky sprites: These tiny, darting spots, visible against smooth blue backgrounds, are white cells moving in the blood vessels over the retina
5 mark 195:
[A long blob, slightly distorting the blue swath.]
Floaters: Some types of floaters are caused by breakdown of your eyeball goop as you age, but this type is some other kind of debris near the retina. I don't know what.
10 mark 270:
[An askew crosshair and circle, with faint blue and yellow wedges inside]
Humans can see polarization - Stare at a white area on an LCD display while rotating it or your head fast (use straight ahead as the axis of rotation).
Polarization direction is shown by a faint central yellow blue shape (Also visible in deep blue skies)
from 0 to 30 mark 340:
[The same image, increasing in absolute size from a very tiny object in the center to one about 20x original size at 30 degrees. The brightness of the image varies from black at 2 mark 340, to gray at 5 mark 340, to nearly white at 10 mark 340, to slightly grayer at 20 mark 340, to medium gray at 30 mark 340.]
Night Vision: Cone cells (sharp, central color vision) don't work in low light, but rod cells (monochrome, low-res, non-central) do. This is why you can walk around in dim light, but not read. It's also why you can spot fainter stars by looking next to them.


comment.png add a comment! ⋅ comment.png add a topic (use sparingly)! ⋅ Icons-mini-action refresh blue.gif refresh comments!

Discussion

I may just print this and laminate it for display. I love it when Randall puts out a poster-size comic because it means more wall decorations. Davidy22[talk] 13:51, 8 January 2013 (UTC)

Anybody else wonder if it's coincidence that a comic about vision - much of its content relating to the resoultion of the eye - is #1080? 108.162.241.17 01:15, 9 August 2014 (UTC)

I typed 1080 into the address bar to see if the number would be relevant to whatever comic loaded. 108.162.237.161 20:00, 16 January 2015 (UTC)

I think the fact this is comic number 1080 is relevant since 1080p is a screen resolution.108.162.216.82 06:41, 23 November 2018 (UTC)